Cultural Geography

(Nora) #1
Humans, animals, spirits, and several geophysical
agents are perceived to have qualities of personhood.
All persons engage in a reciprocally communicative
reality. Human persons are not set over and against a
material context of inert nature, but rather are one
species of person in a network of reciprocating persons.
These reciprocative interactions constitute the events of
experience. (1996: 72–3)

In characterizing what a non-anthropocentric
cosmos looks like, D. Rose discusses the land
ethic of Ngarinman and Ngaliwurru people from
the Northern Territory in Australia. In contrast to
Passmore’s (1995: 140) denial of reciprocity,
Ngarinman people say ‘human life exists within
the broader context of a living and conscious
cosmos’ (D. Rose, 1988: 379). Williams (cited in
Langton, 1998: 27) also argues that ‘Aboriginal
people regard the environment as sentient and as
communicating with them.’ These statements fly
directly in the face of all those knowledges
which are based on a belief in the universal
application of classifications based on the fact
that only humans are conscious beings.
Many Aboriginal people have relationships
with specific species of animals at personal and
tribal scales. These relationships are based on an
underlying understanding that through creation
animals and humans were, are and will be inter-
related. Dreaming stories inform relationships
between humans and animals whereby responsi-
bility to country is based on a common heritage
and kinship (D. Rose; Bennett, 1983): ‘Animals,
they’re related to us ... Animals were human
before’ (Napranum elders in Suchet, 1996: 211).
Many indigenous people in Canada also relate to
‘animals’ in mutually conscious and reciprocal
relationships. In Cree notions of knowing, signing
and making meaning, animals are an integral part
of knowledge systems:

Animal actions, particular qualities and features in the
bodies of animals, weather, dream images and events,
visions, and religious symbols all fall within the Cree
notion of ‘sign’, with signs constituting knowledge or
guidance for actors. Not only humans, but animals and
other nonhuman persons send, interpret and respond to
signs pertinent to various domains of human action:
hunting success or failure, birth and death, and, implicit
to these, the circumstances of reciprocity between
persons in the world. (Scott, 1996: 73)

In Africa, the Ju/’hoansi of Nyae Nyae in north-
eastern Namibia engage in a dialogue with
elephants in which elephants:
must participate in the planning for the harvesting of
Marula[a tree fruit] ... ‘We share the resource with
elephants and we have decided together, that is the
elephants and us, which tree is to be used by the

Ju/’hoansi and which by the elephant.’ (/Ai!ae/Oma,
cited in Powell, 1998: 47)

African discourses also unsettle the naturaliza-
tion of the categories wild, domestic and tame:

Animals have always helped us, and they still do ...
The wild animals and the tame ones are the same to us.
(respected elder Baha Mhlanga from Mahenye, cited in
Hove and Trojanow, 1996: 22–9)

Treating binaries such as society/nature,
human/animal and domestic/wild as self-evident
epistemological givens, naturalizes assertions
and impositions of power and control. The ‘epis-
temological transformation’ (Esteva, 1987: 138)
that occurs when nature, wildlife and ‘wild
humans’ are constructed as resources, legitimates
the assertion and imposition of Eurocentric prac-
tices of management – intervening, taming,
domesticating, controlling, subduing and domi-
nating the wild-wildlife-wilderness. Managerial
intervention, however, is not how all people
interact with worlds. D. Rose (1988) argues that
Ngarinman and Ngaliwurru people are hesitant
to intervene in ecological processes. She notes
that for many people ‘non-intervention is fre-
quently a virtue’. A survey of perceptions of
Aboriginal people in Central Australia similarly
highlights knowledges that are not based on
beliefs in intervention and overt control:
Many people expressed a sense of loss that the [locally
extinct ‘native’] animals were no longer around but
there was also a pervading sense of passive acceptance
about what had happened. Rather than question why the
animals had gone and then attempt to act to bring them
back, Aboriginal people accept what they perceive as a
change in circumstances which is beyond their control.
(B. Rose, 1995: 95)

In Canada, what scientists perceive as population
declines are understood very differently by the
Inuit:
Elders say that any kind of animal moves away for a
while but, according to the government, animals are in
decline. To the Inuit, they have moved, but not
declined. (Peter Alogut, cited in Freeman, 1999: 8)
[M]any Inuit do not believe that ‘wildlife’ can be ‘har-
vested’, ‘managed’ or ‘conserved’ as ‘stocks’ or ‘popu-
lations’. Many of these concepts have no basis in Inuit
reality. (Stevenson, 1996: 8)

Aboriginal people at Napranum on Cape York
Peninsula insist they have their own way of relat-
ing to country, and this may or may not fit into
Eurocentric ideas about what land and resource
management should be. They often express
anger and frustration at not having their ways
recognized and respected by other people and

CONTESTED CULTURAL LANDSCAPES 563

3029-ch31.qxd 03-10-02 11:10 AM Page 563

Free download pdf