Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research and Everyday Experience, 3rd Edition

(Tina Meador) #1

230 • CHAPTER 8 Everyday Memory and Memory Errors


witness’s initially uncertain response turns into an “absolutely
positive” response. In a paper titled “Good, You Identifi ed the
Suspect,” Gary Wells and Amy Bradfi eld (1998) had partici-
pants view a video of an actual crime and then asked them
to identify the perpetrator from a photospread that did not
actually contain a picture of the perpetrator (● Figure 8.21).
All of the participants picked one of the photographs,
and following their choice, witnesses received either con-
fi rming feedback from the experimenter (“Good, you iden-
tifi ed the suspect”), no feedback, or disconfi rming feedback
(“Actually, the suspect was number __”). A short time later,
the participants were asked how confi dent they were about
their identifi cation. The results, shown at the bottom of the
fi gure, indicate that participants who received the confi rm-
ing feedback were more confi dent of their choice.
Wells and Bradfi eld call this increase in confi dence due
to confi rming feedback after making an identifi cation the
post-identifi cation feedback effect. This effect creates a seri-
ous problem in the criminal justice system, because jurors
are strongly infl uenced by how confi dent eyewitnesses are
about their judgments. Thus, faulty eyewitness judgments
can result in picking the wrong person, and the post-
identifi cation feedback effect can increase witnesses’ con-
fi dence that they made the right judgment (Douglass et al.,
2009; Quinlivan et al., 2009; Wells & Quinlivan, 2009).

The Eff ect of Postevent Questioning Wells and
Bradfi eld showed that postevent feedback can strengthen
witnesses’ confi dence in their lineup identifi cation. We will
now describe an experiment by Jason Chan and cowork-
ers (2009) that considers a related question: How does
taking a recall test after witnessing an event and before
being exposed to misleading postevent information infl u-
ence memory for the event? The design of this experiment is shown in ● Figure 8.22.
Participants fi rst viewed a 40-minute episode of the television program 24 , in which
Jack Bauer, played by Kiefer Sutherland, is trying to thwart a terrorist plot. The par-
ticipants were then split into two groups. The test group took a cued recall test about
the video, which contained questions like “What did the terrorist use to knock out
the fl ight attendant?” (correct answers were not provided). The no-test group played
a computer game. Both groups were then given distraction tasks, such as fi lling out a
questionnaire and completing some tests unrelated to the TV program.

●FIGURE 8.22 Design and results of Chan et al.’s (2009) experiment that demonstrated the
reverse testing eff ect. Participants were presented with a distraction task before receiving the
misinformation.

Take cued
recall test Misinformation

Cued
recall test

Play
Tetris

No-test
group

Test group

Misinformation recall testCued

50%

30%

“Yes” response
to incorrect item

View
24
tape

●FIGURE 8.21 Design and results of Wells and Bradfi eld’s
(1998) “Good, You Identifi ed the Suspect” experiment. The type
of feedback from the experimenter infl uenced participants’
confi dence in their identifi cation, with confi rming feedback
resulting in the highest confi dence.

Receive
feedback.

Receive no
feedback.

5.4 4.0 3.5

View video of
crime.

Pick perpetrator
from photospread.

Confidence level rating

Receive
disconfirming
feedback.

Receive
confirming
feedback.

or or

Copyright 2011 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part.
Free download pdf