Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research and Everyday Experience, 3rd Edition

(Tina Meador) #1

236 • CHAPTER 8 Everyday Memory and Memory Errors



  1. True and false memories in the brain. Can we distinguish
    between true and false memories by looking at activ-
    ity in the brain? One idea, called the sensory reactiva-
    tion hypothesis, is that true memories involve activation
    of sensory areas that were activated when the original
    memory was formed.


Schacter, D. L., & Slotnick, S. D. (2004). The cognitive neuro-
science of memory distortion. Neuron, 44, 149–160.

Sederberg, P. B., et al. (2007). Gamma oscillations distinguish true
from false memories. Psychological Science, 18, 927–932.

Wheeler, M. E., & Buckner, R. L. (2004). Functional-anatomic
correlates of remembering and knowing. NeuroImage, 21,
1337–1349.

Wheeler, M. E., Petersen, S. E., & Buckner, R. L. (2000). Mem-
ory’s echo: Vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific
cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
97 , 11125–11129.


  1. Social influence, source monitoring and the misinforma-
    tion effect. Source monitoring and the misinformation
    effect can be influenced by social factors.


Assefi, S. L., & Garry, M. (2003). Absolut® memory distor-
tions: Alcohol placebos influence the misinformation effect.
Psychological Science, 14, 77–80.

Hoffman, H. G., Granhag, P. A., See, S. T. K., & Loftus, E. F.
(2001). Social influences on reality-monitoring decisions.
Memory and Cognition, 29, 394–404.


  1. Memory distortions caused by personal bias. In a very
    readable book, Daniel Schacter describes a number of
    ways that memory can be distorted. In one chapter he
    discusses how memory can be distorted by biases related
    to personal and social factors, such as how people per-
    ceive themselves and how they think about events in
    their lives.
    Schacter, D. L. (2001). The seven sins of memory. New York:
    Houghton Mifflin.

  2. Confabulation. People with damage to their frontal lobes
    often engage in a process called confabulation, which
    involves making outlandish false statements. One char-
    acteristic of confabulation is that the person believes that
    even the most impossible-sounding statements are true. It


has been suggested that this may tell us something about
the role of the frontal lobes in normal memory.

Moscovitch, M. (1995). Confabulation. In D. L. Schacter (Ed.),
Memory distortion (pp. 226–251). Cambridge, MA: Har-
vard University Press.


  1. Mechanisms of the misinformation effect. The chapter
    you just read reviews three ideas about the mechanisms
    responsible for the misinformation effect. This paper
    presents another idea, which argues against the memory
    trace replacement hypothesis.


McCloskey, M., & Zaragoza, Z. (1985). Misleading postevent
information and memory for events: Arguments and
evidence against memory impairment hypothesis. Journal of
Experimental Psychology: General, 114, 3–18.


  1. Suggestibility in children. We have seen that adults can be
    influenced by suggestion. Young children pose additional
    problems, especially when they are called on to present
    testimony in court.


Bruck, M., Ceci, S. J., & Hembrooke, H. (2002). The nature of
children’s true and false narratives. Developmental Review,
22 , 520–554.

Principe, G. F., Kanaya, T., Ceci, S. J., & Singh, M. (2006).
Believing is seeing: How rumors can engender false memo-
ries in preschoolers. Psychological Science, 17, 243–248.


  1. The Moses illusion. The Moses illusion occurs when people
    answer “two” to the question “How many animals of each
    kind did Moses take on the ark?” even though they know
    that Noah was the one with the ark. A number of different
    hypotheses have been proposed to explain this effect.


Park, H., & Reder, L. M. (2004). Moses illusion: Implications
for human cognition. In R. F. Pohl (Ed.), Cognitive illusions
(pp. 275–291). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.


  1. Memory distortions in coerced false confessions. Some
    people who confess to crimes they did not commit come to
    believe that they did commit the crime and can even create
    vivid memories of the crime. The following paper describes
    this striking phenomenon in terms of source monitoring.


Henkel, L. A., & Coffman, K. J. (2004). Memory distortions in
coerced false confessions: A source monitoring framework
analysis. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 10, 567–588.

If You WANT TO KNOW MORE


Amygdala, 208
Autobiographical memory (AM), 205
Cognitive hypothesis, 207
Cognitive interview, 232
Constructive nature of memory, 213


Cryptomnesia, 215
Cultural life script, 207
Cultural life script hypothesis, 207
Eyewitness testimony, 226
Flashbulb memory, 209

Memory trace replacement
hypothesis, 224
Misinformation effect, 222
Misleading postevent information
(MPI), 222

Key TERMS


Copyright 2011 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part.
Free download pdf