Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research and Everyday Experience, 3rd Edition

(Tina Meador) #1

290 • CHAPTER 10 Visual Imagery


Think ABOUT IT



  1. Look at an object for a minute; then look away, create
    a mental image of it, and draw a sketch of the object
    based on your mental image. Then draw a sketch of the
    same object while you are looking at it. How do the two
    sketches differ? What kinds of information about the
    object were you able to include in the sketch that was
    based on your mental image? What information was
    omitted, compared to the sketch you created by looking
    at the object?

  2. Write a description of an object as you are looking at
    it. Then compare the written description with the infor-
    mation you can obtain by looking at the object or at a
    picture of the object. Is it true that “a picture is worth a
    thousand words”? How does your comparison of writ-
    ten and visual representations relate to the discussion


of propositional versus depictive representations in this
chapter?


  1. Try using one of the techniques described at the end of
    this chapter to create images that represent things you
    have to do later today or during the coming week. Then,
    after some time passes (anywhere from an hour to a few
    days), check to see whether you can retrieve the memo-
    ries for these images and if you can remember what they
    stand for.

  2. Describe the connection between the description of the
    visual process in Figure 10.20 and the result of Ganis’s
    fMRI experiment shown in Figure 10.15, in which brain
    activation caused by imagery and by perception was
    compared.


If You WANT TO KNOW MORE



  1. Auditory imagery. Auditory imagery occurs when you
    mentally rehearse a telephone number or when a par-
    ticular song keeps running through your mind. Recent
    research has demonstrated a connection between audi-
    tory imagery and brain activity.


Kraemer, D. J. M., Macrae, C. N., Green, A. E., & Kelly, W. M.
(2005). Sound of silence activates auditory cortex. Nature,
434 , 158.

Zatorre, R. J., Halpern, A. R., Perry, D. W., Meyer, E., & Evans,
A. C. (1996). Hearing in the mind’s ear: A PET investigation
of musical imagery and perception. Journal of Cognitive
Neuroscience, 8, 29–46.


  1. Visual imagery. Although we have discussed visual imag-
    ery extensively in this chapter, it is worth looking at


Stephen Kosslyn’s books on the topic. His latest one is
the following.

Kosslyn, S., Thompson, W. L., & Ganis, G. (2006). The case for
mental imagery. New York: Oxford University Press.


  1. Mechanical reasoning and working memory. The relation-
    ship between solving mechanical problems, imagery,
    and working memory has been studied by determining
    how placing a load on the visuospatial sketch pad com-
    ponent of working memory affects the ability to solve
    these problems. These experiments are analogous to the
    Demonstration on page 135 of Chapter 5.


Sims, V. K., & Hegarty, M. (1997). Mental animation in the
visuospatial sketch pad: Evidence from dual task studies.
Memory & Cognition, 25, 321–333.


  1. Parallels between perception and imagery have been dem-
    onstrated physiologically by the following methods: (a)
    recording from single neurons (imagery neurons); (b)
    brain imaging (demonstrating overlapping activation in
    the brain); (c) transcranial magnetic stimulation experi-
    ments (comparing the effect of brain inactivation on per-
    ception and imagery); and (d) neuropsychological case
    studies (removal of visual cortex affects image size; uni-
    lateral neglect).

  2. There is also physiological evidence for differences
    between imagery and perception. This evidence includes
    (a) differences in areas of the brain activated and
    (b) brain damage causing dissociations between percep-
    tion and imagery.
    8. Most psychologists, taking all of the above evidence into
    account, have concluded that imagery is closely related to
    perception and shares some (but not all) mechanisms.
    9. The use of imagery can improve memory in a number of
    ways: (a) visualizing interacting images; (b) organization
    using the method of loci; and (c) associating items with
    nouns using the pegword technique.
    10. Problems involving mechanical reasoning can be solved
    using either mental simulation or rule-based approaches.
    Experiments with the water-pouring problem show it is
    unlikely that tacit knowledge is involved in using imagery
    to solve this problem. Experiments with the pulley prob-
    lem indicate that people may direct their attention to indi-
    vidual components of the problem, one after the other.


Copyright 2011 Cengage Learning. All Rights Reserved. May not be copied, scanned, or duplicated, in whole or in part.
Free download pdf