321435_Print.indd

(やまだぃちぅ) #1

(Pearman and Storandt 2004 ). They are also associated with negative affect


(Derouesne et al. 1999 ), lower physical functioning (Comijs et al. 2002 ), sleep


disturbance (Weber et al. 2012 ), and stress (Elfgren et al. 2010 ), all of which have


been independently associated with cognitive performance (Reid et al. 2012 ;


Walker 2008 ). Many studies suggest a sex difference in SMC, with women per-


ceiving more memory problems than men (Gagnon et al. 1994 ). Higher levels of


education are associated with fewer SMC and better objective memory performance


(Bassett and Folstein 1993 ). Given the frequency of such complaints, there is great


interest in the possible clinical relevance of SMC as a sign of underlying disease or


change in brain function, but their significance is not entirely known. The literature


addressing this issue is mixed, and questions remain about whether or not indi-


viduals are generally able to accurately assess their own memory abilities.


In middle-aged adults, SMC are generally thought to reflect underlying psycho-


logical dysfunction, such as depression or anxiety, sleep disturbance, or stress, rather


than neurologic dysfunction. This is because memory disorders such as Alzheimer’s


disease are very rare atmidlife.Itis now wellrecognizedthat psychological dysfunction


negativelyaffectsmemoryandothercognitivefunctions.Further,theneuropathological


processes that characterize Alzheimer’s disease begin at midlife, a decade or more


before the onset of dementia. Thus, for individuals at midlife and beyond, there is


concern that SMC may reflect the beginning of a true decline in cognitive and/or brain
function. The menopausal transition is often accompanied by both subjective and ob-


jective changes in memory function (Mitchell and Woods 2001 ; Greendale et al. 2009 ;


Eppersonet al. 2013 ), withnearlytwo-thirdsof womenreporting anundesirable change


inmemoryfunction(Mitchell and Woods 2001 ).Ingeneral,there are few investigations


that have systematically compared SMC with concurrent objective memory perfor-


mance in young and middle-aged adults. However, memory changes are thought to


occur across all of adulthood (Zacks et al. 1999 ).


In older adults, SMC are often thought to be an early marker of cognitive decline


or dementia. They are a core feature of the diagnostic criteria for mild cognitive


impairment (MCI), an intermediate condition between normal aging and dementia.


This criterion assumes that SMC can serve as a proxy for objective memory decline.


Data supporting this assumption is mixed, with some studies demonstrating that


individuals with SMC perform more poorly on objective memory tests than those


without complaints, whereas other studies demonstrate that SMC relate only to


depression and other forms of psychological distress (see Reid and MacLullich


2006 ; Roberts et al. 2009 ; Stewart 2012 for reviews). SMC have also been shown to


relate to cognitive functions other than memory, such as attention (Scholtissen-In de


Braek et al. 2011 ; Weber et al. 2012 ; Drogos et al. 2013 ). Neuroimaging studies


show a relationship between SMC and brain structure and function (see Stewart


2012 for review). However, many patients with Alzheimer’s disease fail to rec-


ognize their cognitive declines and deny memory problems. Thus, SMC are used as


13 Subjective Memory Complaints and Objective Memory Performance 277

Free download pdf