The higher densities of rodent populations indi-
cated by the crowding effect are an important part of
this analysis. As already highlighted, because islands
tend to have fewer species on them than equivalent
continental areas, population densities will differ
between islands and mainlands, in effect as a statis-
tical artefact. Williamson (1981) therefore distin-
guishes the notions of density compensationand
density stasis. Density compensation is where
island communities have the same total population
density but distributed over fewer species, i.e. with
large population sizes per species (above). Density
statis is where the overall population of the commu-
nity on the island is less than that of the reference
mainland system, such that population sizes per
species are the same as the mainland. Distinguishing
between the two conditions is tricky and necessitates
an appropriate experimental design.
According to Adler and Levins (1994) a number of
studies of island rodents have avoided the pitfalls
identified by Williamson, demonstrating that higher
average densities of rodent populations do indeed
occur in some cases on islands. They cite several
examples of experimental studies using fenced
enclosures, which have shown that rodent popula-
tions in such circumstances can reach abnormally
high densities and often destroy the food supply
(the ‘fence effect’ of Table 7.3.), thereby indicating
dispersal to be an important regulator of population
densities under normal circumstances in which
there is no (complete) fence. As rodent populations
on small islands lack an adequate dispersal sink, the
fence effect may be invoked as a force providing
selective influence on the population.
One of the mainstays of island ecologicalanalysis
in recent decades has been the study of the relative
significance of island area and isolation (Chapter 4).
Adler and Levins’ (1994) scheme extends to dealing
with how these factors may work in an island evo-
lutionarycontext (see also Adler et al. 1986). They
hypothesize that average population density
might, in general, increase with increasing isolation
and hence with reduced dispersal, but that this
fence effect might decline with an increase of island
area to the point where it disappears altogether as
islands more closely resemble a mainland. Thus the
island syndrome might be expected to occur only in
islands which are: (1) sufficiently isolated, (2) not
too large so as to resemble mainland, and (3) not so
small that they cannot support persistent popula-
tions. Figure 7.7 illustrates the differing effects that
area and isolation are envisaged to have. Effects
which may be expressed largely as reaction norms
in the short term i.e. within the range of phenotypes
of the founding population, may, under sustained
selection pressure, produce rapid evolutionary
change and locally adapted island populations that
differ from mainland populations. The effect of iso-
lation is a direct one, on dispersal, whereas that of
area is less direct. Larger islands and mainlands
typically have more predators, competitors, and
habitat types, and because of this (and especially
because of predation effects), densities are
depressed compared with those of small islands.
NICHE SHIFTS AND SYNDROMES 193
REDUCED
DISPERSAL
GREATER
NEIGHBOUR
FAMILIARITY
REDUCED
AGGRESSION
MORE
STABLE
SOCIAL
STRUCTURE
GREATER
POPULATION
STABILITY
FOUNDER
POPULATION
BETTER
SURVIVAL
REDUCED
PREDATION
HIGHER
DENSITY
REDUCED
REPRODUCTIVE
EFFORT
GREATER
BODY
SIZE
Isolation
Reduced
area
Figure 7.7Schematic diagram showing the initial effects of island isolation and area on rodent populations. Long-term effects of insularity are
directional selection for increased body size, reduced reproductive output, and reduced aggression (Redrawn from Adler and Levins 1994, Fig. 2).