Consciousness

(Tuis.) #1

  • seCtIon FoUR: eVoLUtIon
    of what another chimp can see. They might have
    an evolved tendency to look where someone else
    is looking. To find out, careful experiments are
    needed.
    Chimpanzees beg for food from humans and from
    each other. In an ingenious series of experiments,
    Povinelli (1998) and his colleagues used this
    behaviour to find out whether chimpanzees know
    what someone else can see. First they tested the
    chimps to make sure that they begged for food
    from an experimenter out of their reach, and did
    not beg for inedible items. Then two experiment-
    ers offered them food; one had a blindfold over
    her mouth and the other had one over her eyes.
    The chimps came into the lab, paused, and then
    begged for the food, but they were just as likely to
    gesture to the person who could not see them as
    the one who could. This was even true when one
    experimenter had a bucket over her head. Some-
    times, when their begging failed to elicit any food,
    they begged again, as though puzzled at getting
    no response.
    They seemed to pass one test: when one person turned her back the chimpan-
    zees were less likely to gesture to her. However, when both experimenters sat
    with their backs to the apes and one looked back over her shoulder, the chimpan-
    zees gestured randomly to both. They seemed oblivious to the fact that there is
    no point begging to someone who cannot see you. This is dramatically different
    from the behaviour of human children, who can understand this before they are
    three years old.
    More recently, researchers have devised tests for theory of mind that do not
    require cooperation with humans. One experiment investigated whether subor-
    dinate chimps understand what their dominant counterparts know about food
    they are competing for. The researchers found that the subordinates can distin-
    guish between cases where the dominant has or has not seen the food hidden
    or moved: subordinates will go for food unseen by the dominant competitor, but
    stay away when they know it has been seen (Hare, Call, and Tomasello, 2001).
    Experiments have also distinguished between knowing about others’ knowledge
    and about their beliefs. Two chimps take turns choosing from a row of buckets,
    some of them containing food. In the first condition (the knowledge–ignorance
    test), one chimp sees its competitor observing one of two pieces of food being
    hidden and then choosing one of three buckets. Can the chimp use its knowl-
    edge of what its competitor knows to determine which bucket might still contain
    food? In the second condition, the chimp sees its competitor misled by an exper-
    imenter pretending to put food in one bucket. Can it predict the competitor’s
    choice (by identifying their false belief about the location of the food)? Six-year-
    old children pass both tests, but the chimps fail the false-belief test (Kaminski, Call
    and Tomasello, 2008). Some have speculated that apes can represent relations
    between agents and information that is true from their perspective, but cannot


‘Unless one needs to


discuss behaviour, or to


catch a Hollywood spy,


submentalising may be


the smart option’


(Heyes, 2017, p. 2)


FIGURE 10.11 • Deception and theory of mind
are closely linked. Only a
creature capable of attributing
mental states to others would
hope to get away with illicit
activity by hiding behind a rock.

Free download pdf