Consciousness

(Tuis.) #1

  • seCtIon FIVe: BoRDeRLAnDs
    In these ways, the various physiological states of
    sleep can be recognised and studied, but what
    about the experience? Approximately 14% of
    people report dreaming every night, 25% report
    dreaming frequently, and 6% never, and dream
    recall decreases with age (Blagrove, 2009). The
    emphasis here is on recall rather than dreaming
    itself because most dreams are never recalled.
    This was discovered in the 1950s when EEG
    studies first revealed the stages of sleep and
    people could be woken selectively at different
    stages.
    When woken from non-REM sleep, people
    typically say either that nothing was going on
    in their mind or that they were thinking. As a
    simple example: ‘I was asleep. I wasn’t thinking
    about anything or dreaming about anything.’
    Or ‘I was thinking about my nephew. It’s his
    birthday soon and I  must send him a card’.
    Non-REM reports are usually short and lacking
    in detail.
    By contrast, when woken from REM sleep, people typically report that they were
    having complex, much longer, and often bizarre dreams; sometimes very bizarre,
    as in this excerpt:


I was at a conference and trying to get breakfast but the food and the
people in line kept changing. My legs didn’t work properly and I found it
a great effort to hold my tray up. Then I realised why. My body was rotting
away, and liquid was oozing from it. I thought I might be completely
rotted before the end of the conference, but I thought I should still get
some coffee if I had the strength.

We cannot say that this is a typical dream, for there is probably no such thing, but
it has familiar elements that most people will probably recognise, especially the
matter-of-fact response to profound bizarreness. The contents of dreams have
been thoroughly studied using questionnaires and interviews, and by analysing
reports using a scoring system originally developed in the 1960s by Calvin Hall
and Robert van de Castle (1966; Domhoff, 1996). This counts such elements as set-
tings, characters, emotions, social interactions, and misfortunes, all of which show
remarkable consistency across times and cultures, with reliable sex differences,
and reliable differences between the dreams of adults and children (Domhoff,
1996, Ch. 4).
For example, men dream more about other men than women do, and have more
aggressive interactions. Children, by contrast, dream more often about animals,
suffer more dream misfortunes, and are more often the victim of aggression than
its initiator. Specific emotions or moods occur in about three quarters of dreams
and are roughly equally positive and negative. Joy is the most common emotion
reported, followed by anger and fear. Events in waking life often play a role in

Behaviour

Awake

Polygraph

EMG
EEG
EOG

Sensation and
perception

Thought

Movement

WAKE NREM SLEEP REM SLEEP

Stages REM

Vivid,
externally
generated

Dull or absent

Vivid,
internally
generated
Logical
progressive

Logical
perseverative

Illogical
bizarre

Continuous
voluntary

Episodic
involuntary

Commanded
but inhibited

II
III
IV

I

dreams, including dreams about such life events
as surgery, psychotherapy, or marriage and
divorce, as well as trivial events of the previous
day.
If we wake up with a memory of dreaming, we are
likely to try to make sense of what we dreamed;
indeed, the sense-making process is part of the
remembering. The natural tendency to attribute
significance to dreams (perhaps even more than
to events in waking life; Morewedge and Norton,
2009) was encouraged by Freud’s (1900/1999)
psychoanalytic approach to dream interpreta-
tion, which treats them as forms of wish fulfilment
in which the real (or ‘latent’) content, deriving
from the unconscious, is disguised in the super-
ficial ‘manifest’ content of the dream scenarios.
Jung (e.g. 1934–1936/1968) adapted these ideas
to emphasise the role of basic archetypes that
represent unconscious attitudes, and can be
manifested in various dream symbols and figures
which take dynamic forms depending on the
dreamer and the dream context. Neither of these
theories has stood the test of time. Although
dream interpretation books and websites offer-
ing readymade templates for meaning-making
are popular and many people believe their
dreams give insight into unconscious beliefs and
desires, there is no good reason to think that they
do more than reflect current worries or hopes.
There are problems with generalising about
dream content because of the effects of the
method of collecting reports. For example, some
researchers have asked people to keep dream
diaries with dreams collected over long periods,
while others ask just for the most recent dream. Selective reporting can be a prob-
lem with all collection methods, however, and the selection may take place at
several stages: only some dreams are recalled on waking, some fade faster from
memory after waking, and further selection can occur when people are asked to
write a report or describe their dreams. In consequence, the occurrence of bizarre
or interesting dreams may be exaggerated. Certainly many dreams are bizarre,
but in studies that try to avoid selection problems, bizarreness is found in only
about 10% of dreams.
This bizarreness takes different forms. Allan Hobson (1999) suggested three cat-
egories: incongruity involves the mismatching of features of characters, objects,
actions, or settings; discontinuity involves sudden changes in these elements;
uncertainty involves explicit vagueness. Research from his group suggested that
the way characters and objects are transformed in dreams follows certain rules
but that changes of scene and plot do not. Perhaps the strangest thing about

‘As for dreams – they’re


the “B-movies” of the


mind – entertaining, but


best forgotten’


(Horne, 2009, p. 709)


FIGURE 15.1 • Behavioural states in humans.
States of waking, NREM sleep,
and REM sleep have behavioural,
polygraphic, and psychological
manifestations. The sequence of
these stages is represented in
the polygraph channel. Sample
tracings of three variables used
to distinguish state are also
shown: electromyogram (EMG),
which is highest in waking,
intermediate in NREM sleep, and
lowest in REM sleep; and the
electroencephalogram (EEG) and
electro-oculogram (EOG), which
are both activated in waking
and REM sleep and inactivated
in NREM sleep. Each sample
is approximately 20 seconds
(Hobson 2002, Figure 2;
Hobson, 2009, p. 805).

Free download pdf