The Psychology of Gender 4th Edition

(Tuis.) #1
74 Chapter 3

(Glick et al., 2000). Among those countries,
nations that scored higher in hostile and be-
nevolent sexism also scored higher in gender
inequality, as measured by the presence of
women in politics, the number of women in
the workforce, and female literacy rates.

Sexism Toward Men


Although sexism can be exhibited toward
both women and men, it is typically studied
and measured as feelings toward women.
Jokes about female drivers and “dumb
blondes” are regularly perceived as exam-
ples of sexism. But aren’t jokes about men’s
incompetence at being fathers or men
not asking for directions also examples of

Although the benevolent and hostile
sexism scales reflect two very different affec-
tive states in regard to women, the two are
positively correlated, meaning that people
who endorse items on one scale also endorse
items on the other. Perceiving women in both
negative and positive terms seems contradic-
tory. The ambivalence in attitudes toward
women stems from the paradox that women
hold a lower status than men, but that the fe-
male stereotype is more positive than the male
stereotype. This positive correlation under-
scores the idea that both hostile sexism and
benevolent sexism are based on a belief that
women are inferior to men. The positive cor-
relation of the benevolent and hostile sexism
scales has been shown to exist in 19 countries

SIDEBAR 3.1:Benevolent Sexism and Female Criminals


Are women and men treated equally within the criminal justice system? Some believe that women
are treated more leniently than men partly because women are viewed as less of a threat to society
than men (weaker) and partly because of a paternalistic need to help and care for women. However,
when women commit crimes that violate the female stereotype, they could be treated more harshly.
In a study of a local newspaper in Bloomington, Indiana, women who committed violent crimes
were treated more harshly than men by the media, whereas women who committed nonviolent
crimes were treated more leniently by the media (Grabe et al., 2006). In another study where regis-
tered voters posed as mock jurors, women received a lighter sentence than men for a heinous crime
unless there was testimony from the victim’s family—in which case, the female received a more
severe sentence than the male (Forsterlee et al., 2004). Forsterlee and colleagues argue that the tes-
timony made the incongruence between such extreme violence and the female gender role salient.
One example of an extreme violation of the female gender role is killing children. In 1966,
Myra Hindley tortured and murdered five children. She was not declared mentally ill and was sen-
tenced to life in prison. When a group of young adults, mostly college students, were presented
with this information, those who scored high on benevolent sexism judged Myra more harshly than
those who scored lower on the scale (Viki, Massey, & Masser, 2005). Those who scored higher on
benevolent sexism also were more likely to say that Myra violated the female gender role, and this
gender-role violation explained the link between benevolent sexism and the negative evaluation of
Myra. Neither sex nor hostile sexism was related to evaluations of Myra. Thus in this case, benevo-
lent sexism was related to a negative judgment rather than a positive judgment of a woman.

M03_HELG0185_04_SE_C03.indd 74 6/21/11 12:22 PM

Free download pdf