Baumeister, R. F., & Cairns, K. J. (1992). Repression and self-presentation: When audiences in-
terfere with self-deceptive strategies.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62 ,
851–862.
Berzonsky, M. D., & Sullivan, C. (1992). Social-cognitive aspects of identity style: Need for cog-
nition, experiential openness, and introspection.Journal of Adolescent Research, 7 , 140–155.
Blanchard-Fields, F. (1994). Age differences in causal attributions from an adult developmental
perspective.Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences & Social Sciences, 49 ,
P43–P51.
Blanchard-Fields, F. (1999). Social schemacity and causal attributions. In T. M. Hess & F.
Blanchard-Fields (Eds.),Social cognition and aging(pp. 219–236). San Diego, CA: Academic
Press.
Blanchard-Fields, F., Baldi, R., & Stein, R. (1999). Age relevance and context effects on attribu-
tions across the adult life span.International Journal of Behavioral Development, 23 , 665–683.
Blanchard-Fields, F., & Norris, L. (1994). Causal attributions from adolescence through adult-
hood: Age differences, ego level, and generalized response style.Aging, Neuropsychology, and
Cognition, 1 , 67–86.
Bowlby, J. (1978). Attachment theory and its therapeutic implications.Adolescent Psychiatry, 6 ,
5–33.
Bowlby, J. (1989). The role of attachment in personality development and psychopathology. In
S. I. Greenspan & G. H. Pollock (Eds.),The course of life, Vol. 1: Infancy(pp. 229–270).
Madison, CT: International Universities Press.
Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1999). Affective norms for English words (ANEW). Gainesville,
FL: The NIMH Center for the Study of Emotions and Attention, University of Florida.
Brent, S. B. (1978). Motivation, steady-state, and structural development: A general model of
psychological homeostasis.Motivation and Emotion, 2 , 299–332.
Bruner, J. (1986).Actual minds, possible worlds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: The HAROLD model.
Psychology and Aging, 17 , 85–100.
Cacioppo, J. T., & Petty, R. E. (1982). The need of cognition.Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 42 , 116–131.
Cacioppo, J. T., Petty, R. E., Feinstein, J., & Jarvis, W. (1996). Dispositional differences in cog-
nitive motivation: The life and times of individuals varying in need for cognition.Psychologi-
cal Bulletin, 119 , 197–253.
Carstensen, L. L. (1991). Socioemotional selectivity theory: Social activity in life-span context.
Annual Review of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 11 , 195–217.
Carstensen, L. L. (1993). Motivation for social contact across the life span: A theory of
socioemotional selectivity. In J. E. Jacobs (Ed.),Nebraska symposium on motivation, 1992:
Developmental perspectives on motivation(pp. 209–254). Lincoln, NE: University of Ne-
braska Press.
Carstensen, L. L., Gross, J. J., & Fung, H. H. (1998). The social context of emotional experience.
In K. W. Schaie & M. P. Lawton (Eds.),Annual review of gerontology and geriatrics, Vol. 17:
Focus on emotion and adult development(pp. 325–352). New York: Springer.
Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., & Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: A theory of
sociomotional selectivity.American Psychologist, 54 , 165–181.
Carver, C. S., & Scheier, M. F. (1995). The role of optimism versus pessimism in the experience
of the self. In A. Oosterwegel & R. A. Wicklund (Eds.),The self in European and North Amer-
ican culture: Development and processes(pp. 193–204). New York: Plenum.
Chaiken, S., & Trope, Y. (Eds.). (1999).Dual-process theories in social psychology. New York:
Guilford.
Clore, G. L., & Ortony, A. (2000). Cognition in emotion: Always, sometimes, or never? In R. D.
Lane & L. Nadel (Eds.),Cognitive neuroscience of emotion(pp. 24–61). New York: Oxford
University Press.
- AFFECT OPTIMIZATION AND DIFFERENTIATION 265