Dewey, J. (1913).Interest and effort in education. Boston: Riverside Press.
Dewey, J. (1916).Democracy and education. New York: The Free Press.
Dewey, J. (1933). The process and product of reflective activity: Psychological process and logi-
cal forms. In J. Boydston (Ed.),The later works of John Dewey(Vol. 8, pp. 171–186). Carbon-
dale, IL: Southern Illinois University Press.
Drevets, W. C., & Raichle, M. E. (1998). Reciprocal suppression of regional cerebral blood flow
during emotional versus higher cognitive processes: Implications for interactions between
emotion and cognition.Cognition and Emotion, 12, 353–385.
Dweck, C. S. (1999).Self theories: Their role in motivation, personality, and development.Phila-
delphia: Psychology Press.
Edelman, G. M. (1989).The remembered present: A biological theory of consciousness. New
York: Basic Books.
Edelman, G. M. (1995). Memory and the individual soul: Against silly reductionism. In J.
Cornwell (Ed.),Nature’s imagination: The frontiers of scientific vision(pp. 200–206). Oxford,
England: Oxford University Press.
Eich, E., Kihlstrom, J. F., Bower, G. H., Forgas, J. P., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2000).Cognition and
emotion. New York: Oxford University Press.
Einstein, A. (1971).Barn–Einstein correspondence. New York: Walk.
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Romer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the
acquisition of expert performance.Psychological Review, 100, 363–406.
Estes, W. K. (1986). Where is intelligence? In R. J. Sternberg & D. K. Detterman (Eds.),What is
intelligence? Contemporary viewpoints on its nature and definition(pp. 63–67). Norwood, NJ:
Ablex.
Ferrari, M., & Elik, N. (2003). Influences on intentional conceptual change. In G. M. Sinatra &
P. R. Pintrich (Eds.),Intentional conceptual change(pp. 21–54). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence
Erlbaum Associates.
Festinger, L. (1957).A theory of cognitive dissonance theory. Stanford, CA: Stanford University
Press.
Ford, M. E. (1992).Motivating humans: Goals, emotions, and personal agency beliefs. Newbury,
CT: Sage.
Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM).Psychological Bulle-
tin, 117, 39–66.
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions?Review of General Psychology, 2,
300–319.
Galton, F. (1883).Inquiries into human faculty and its development. London: Dent.
Gardner, H. (1985).The mind’s new science: A history of the cognitive revolution. New York: Ba-
sic Books.
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2000). Cerebral specialization and interhemispheric communication: Does the
corpus callosum enable the human condition?Brain, 123, 1293–1326.
Geary, D. (1995). Reflections of evolution and culture in children’s cognition.American Psychol-
ogist, 50, 24–37.
Glenberg, A. M. (1997). Mental models, space, and embodied cognition. In T. B. Ward, S. M.
Smith, & J. Vaid (Eds.),Creative thought: An investigation of conceptual structures and proc-
esses(pp. 495–522). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Goldin, G. A. (2000). Affective pathways and representation in mathematical problem solving.
Mathematical Thinking and Learning: An International Journal, 2, 209–219.
Greeno, J. G. (1989). A perspective on thinking.American Psychologist, 44, 134–141.
Greeno, J. G. (1991). Number sense as situated knowing in a conceptual domain.Journal for Re-
search in Mathematics Education, 22, 170–218.
Greenough, W. T., Black, J. E., & Wallace, C. S. (1987). Experience and brain development.
Child Development, 58, 539–559.
- BEYOND COGNITIVISM 33