Handbook of Psychology, Volume 4: Experimental Psychology

(Axel Boer) #1
The“Structure” of the Visual Environment and Perception115

Thestimulusconsistsofasetofalignedlinesegments
(arrangedinthisexampleinanearlyverticalpathtotherightof
theverticalmidline),embeddedinthebackgroundofrandomly
orientedlinesegments.Observersviewedsuccessivepresenta-
tionsoftwopatterns,onecontainingboththetargetpathand
thebackgroundnoiseandonecontainingonlythebackground
noise.Theirorderwasrandomized.Thetaskwastotellwhich
ofthetwopresentationscontainedthetargetpath.
Geisler et al. (2001) varied the length of the path, the
amplitude of the path deviations from a straight line, and path
noisiness (due to the range of random orientations of the line
segments comprising the path) to generate up to 216 classes
of random contour shape. The data from the psychophysical
experiments provided the authors with a “detailed parametric
measurement of human ability to detect naturalistic contours
in noisy backgrounds” (p. 717).
To generate the predictions of contour grouping from the
EC statistics, Geisler et al. (2001) needed a function that
determines which pairs of edge elements group together.
Theauthors derived two such local grouping functions(Fig-
ures4.19E and 4.19F)—one based on the absolute statistic
and one based on the Bayesian statistic—which we explore in
detail in a moment. Because Geisler et al. measured EC for
pairs of edge elements, they used a transitivity rule to con-
struct contours consisting of more than two elements: “if
edge element abinds to b, and bbinds to c, then abecomes
bound to c.” Using this rule, Geisler et al. could predict which
target paths are seen in their stimuli, using the local grouping
functions derived from the statistics of natural images: An
example of grouping by continuation from image statistics
isshown in Figure 4.21B. We consider the Bayesian local
grouping function first, because it requires fewer parameters
than does the absolute local grouping function.


Bayesian Local Grouping Function


Aswesawearlier,thelikelihoodratioateverylocationinthe
(d,)spaceinFigure4.19Dtells,for36orientationdiffer-
ences,howlikelyitisthattheedgeelementsbelongtothe


samecontourasthereferenceelement.Todecidewhethertwo
edgeelementsbelongtothesamecontour,foranyparticular
relationshipd,,betweentheelements,thecorresponding
likelihoodratiocanbecomparedwithacriterion,which
Geisleretal.(2001)calledabindingcriterion,.Asthesig-
naldetectiontheoryprescribes,theidealbindingcriterionis
equaltotheratioofpriorprobabilities(calledpriorodds,as
discussedearlier):

(4)

wherep(C) and p(~C) are the probabilities of two edge ele-
ments to belong or not to belong, respectively, to the same
contour.
TheprioroddswereavailabletoGeisleretal.(2001)di-
rectlyfromtheBayesianECstatistic.Inatrueidealobserver
modelofgroupingbygoodcontinuation,thelocalgrouping
functionwouldhavetocompletelydeterminewhichedgeele-
mentsshouldgroupsolelyfromthestatisticsofnaturalim-
ages(i.e.,withnofreeparametersinthemodel).However,it
turnedoutthatGeisleretal.couldnotusethisoptimalstrategy
becausetheyfoundthatthemagnitudeofvariedasthey
variedtheareaofanalysisintheimage.Inotherwords,the
authorscouldnotfindaunique—anideal—magnitudeof.
Instead,Geisleretal.decidedtoleaveasa(single)freepa-
rameterintheirmodel,justastheobservercriterionisafree
parameterinmodelinghumandataobtainedinadetectionex-
periment.Byfittingthesinglefree-parametermodeltohuman
data,Geisleretal.foundthatthebestresultsareachievedwith
=0.38;theBayesianlocalgroupingfunctionshowninFig-
ure 4.19F was constructed using that best-fitting magnitude of
. Thus, the local grouping function was not truly ideal.

Absolute Local Grouping Function

Because absolute EC statistics do not convey information
about belongingness of edge elements to contours, Geisler
et al. (2001) had to introduce a second parameter, in addition
to binding criterion , in order to derive a local grouping
function from the absolute EC statistics. This new parameter,
calledtolerance,determined how sharply the probabilities of
element grouping fell off around the most likely parameters
of EC shown in Figure 4.19C. For example, low tolerance
implies that grouping occurs only when the parameters
are close to the most common values evident in the absolute
EC statistics. Different values of tolerance result in different
absolute local grouping functions; one is shown in Fig-
ure 4.19E. When fitting the predictions of the two-parameter
absolute local grouping functions to human data, Geisler et
al. were able to obtain almost as good a correlation between
the predicted and the observed accuracies (r=.87) as they

^1 p(C)


p(C)

p(C)


p(C)

Figure 4.21 (A) An example of the path stimulus. (B) The prediction of
grouping in A, from the EC statistics shown in Figure 4.19. Source:Copy-
right 2001 by Elsevier Science Ltd. Reprinted with permission.


[Image not available in this electronic edition.]
Free download pdf