Handbook of Psychology, Volume 4: Experimental Psychology

(Axel Boer) #1

350 Motor Control


Kelso, J. A. S. (1984). Phase transitions and critical behavior
in human bimanual coordination. American Journal of Physiol-
ogy: Regulatory, Integrative, and Comparative, 246,R1000–
R1004.
Kelso, J. A. S. (1994). Elementary coordination dynamics. In S. P.
Swinnen, H. Heuer, J. Massion, & P. Casaer (Eds.), Interlimb
coordination: Neural, dynamical, and cognitive constraints
(pp. 301–318). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.


Kelso, J. A. S., Cook, E., Olson, M. E., & Epstein, W. (1975). Allo-
cation of attention and the locus of adaptation to displaced
vision.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception
and Performance, 1,237–245.
Kelso, J. A. S., & Holt, K. G. (1980). Exploring a vibratory system
analysis of human movement production. Journal of Neurophys-
iology, 43,1183–1196.


Kelso, J. A. S., Southard, D. L., & Goodman, D. (1979). On the
coordination of two-handed movements. Journal of Experimen-
tal Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 5,229–
238.
Kelso, J. A. S., Tuller, B., & Harris, K. S. (1983). A “dynamic
pattern” perspective on the control and coordination of move-
ment. In P. F. MacNeilage (Ed.), The production of speech
(pp. 137–173). Berlin, Germany: Springer.


Kelso, J. A. S., Tuller, B., Vatikiotis-Bateson, E., & Fowler, C. A.
(1984). Functionally specific articulatory cooperation following
jaw perturbation during speech: Evidence for coordinative struc-
tures.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception
and Performance, 10,812–832.
Klapp, S. T. (1979). Doing two things at once: The role of temporal
compatibility. Memory and Cognition, 7,375–381.


Klapp, S. T. (1981). Temporal compatibility in dual motor tasks II:
simultaneous articulation and hand movements. Memory and
Cognition, 9,398–401.
Klapp, S. T., Hill, M., Tyler, J., Martin, Z., Jagacinski, R., & Jones,
M. (1985). On marching to two different drummers: Perceptual
aspects of the difficulties.Journal of Experimental Psychology:
Human Perception and Performance, 11,814–828.
Klemm, O. (1938). Zwölf Leitsätze zu einer Psychologie der
Leibesübungen [Twelve guiding principles for a psychology of
gymnastics].Neue Psychologische Studien, 9,351–382.


Koh, K., & Meyer, D. E. (1991). Function learning: Induction of con-
tinuous stimulus-response relations.Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 17,811–836.
Kohler, I. (1964). The formation and transformation of the percep-
tual world. Psychological Issues, 3,1–173.


Kornhuber, H. H., & Deecke, L. (1965). Hirnpotentialänderungen
bei Willkürbewegungen und passiven Bewegungen des Men-
schen: Bereitschaftspotential und reafferente Potentiale
[Changes of brain potentials in voluntary and passive move-
ments of man: Bereitschaftspotential and reafferente potentiale].
Pflügers Archiv für die gesamte Physiologie, 284,1–17.


Krakauer, J. W., Ghilardi, M. F., & Ghez, C. (1999). Independent
learning of internal models for kinematic and dynamic control of
reaching.Nature Neuroscience, 2,1026–1031.
Krampe, R. T., Kliegl, R., Mayr, U., Engbert, R., & Vorberg, D.
(2000). The fast and the slow of skilled bimanual rhythm produc-
tion: Parallel versus integrated timing.Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26,206–233.
Kravitz, J. H. (1972). Conditioned adaptation to prismatic displace-
ment.Perception & Psychophysics, 11,38–42.
Laabs, G. J. (1974). The effect of interpolated motor activity on the
short-term retention of movement distance and end-location.
Journal of Motor Behavior, 6, 279–288.
Lajoie, Y., Paillard, J., Teasdale, N., Bard, C., Fleury, M., Forget, R.,
& Lamarre, Y. (1992). Mirror drawing in a deafferented patient
and normal subjects: Visuoproprioceptive conflict.Neurology,
42,1104–1106.
Lashley, K. S. (1917). The accuracy of movement in the absence of
excitation from the moving organ. American Journal of Physiol-
ogy, 43,169–194.
Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In
L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanism in behavior(pp. 112–
131). New York: Wiley.
Latash, M. L., & Jaric, S. (in press). The organization of drinking:
Postural characteristics of the arm-head coordination. Journal of
Motor Behavior.
Lee, D. N. (1976). A theory of visual control of braking based on
information about time-to-collision. Perception, 5,437–459.
Lee, T. D., Blandin, Y., & Proteau, L. (1996). Effects of task in-
structions and oscillation frequency on bimanual coordination.
Psychological Research, 59,100–106.
Legge, D. (1965). Analysis of visual and proprioceptive compo-
nents of motor skill by means of a drug. British Journal of Psy-
chology, 56,243–254.
Lépine, D., Glencross, D., & Requin, J. (1989). Some experimental
evidence for and against a parametric conception of movement
programming.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance, 15,347–362.
Lippold, O. C. J. (1952). The relation between integrated action
potentials in a human muscle and its isometric tension. Journal
of Physiology, 117,492–499.
Lippold, O. C. J., Redfearn, J. W. T., & Vucˇo, j. (1960). The elec-
tromyography of fatigue. Ergonomics, 3,121–131.
Mack, A., Heuer, F., Villardi, K., & Chambers, D. (1985). The
dissociation of position and extent in Müller-Lyer figures.
Perception & Psychophysics, 37,335–344.
MacKenzie, C. L., Marteniuk, R. G., Dugas, C., Liske, D., &
Eickmeier, B. (1987). Three-dimensional movement trajectories
in Fitts’ task: Implications for control. Quarterly Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 39A,629–647.
Marteniuk, R. G., & MacKenzie, C. L. (1980). A preliminary theory
of two-hand co-ordinated control. In G. E. Stelmach & J. Requin
Free download pdf