Handbook of Psychology, Volume 4: Experimental Psychology

(Axel Boer) #1
References 351

(Eds.),Tutorials in motor behavior(pp. 185–197). Amsterdam:
North-Holland.
Marteniuk, R. G., MacKenzie, C. L., & Baba, D. M. (1984). Biman-
ual movement control: Information processing and interaction
effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 36A,
335–365.


Marteniuk, R. G., MacKenzie, C. L., Jeannerod, M., Athènes, S., &
Dugas, C. (1987). Constraints on human arm movement trajec-
tories.Canadian Journal of Psychology, 41,365–378.
Massion, J. (1992). Movement, posture, and equilibrium: Interac-
tions and coordination.Progress in Neurobiology, 38,35–56.
McDowell, M. J., & Wolff, P. H. (1997). A functional analysis of
human mirror movements. Journal of Motor Behavior, 29,
85–96.


McGonigle, B. O., & Flook, J. (1978). Long-term retention of single
and multistate prismatic adaptation by human. Nature, 272,
364–366.
McIntyre, J., Stratta, F., & Lacquaniti, F. (1998). Short-term mem-
ory for reaching to visual targets: Psychophysical evidence for
body-centered reference frames. Journal of Neuroscience, 18,
8423–8435.
McLeod, P., McLaughlin, C., & Nimmo-Smith, I. (1985). Informa-
tion encapsulation and automaticity: Evidence from the visual
control of finely timed actions. In M. I. Posner & O. S. M. Marin
(Eds.),Attention and performance XI(pp. 391–406). Hillsdale,
NJ: Erlbaum.
Merz, F., Kalveram, K.-T., & Huber, K. (1981). Der Einfluß kogni-
tiver Faktoren auf Steuerleistungen [The role of cognitive factors
in manual tracking]. In L. Tent (Ed.), Erkennen–Wollen–
Handeln(pp. 327–335). Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe.


Metz, A. M. (1970). Änderungen der myoelektrischen Aktivität
während eines sensomotorischen Lernprozesses [Changes of
myoelectric activity during motor learning]. Zeitschrift für Psy-
chologie, 178,51–88.
Meyer, D. E., Abrams, R. A., Kornblum, S., Wright, C. E., & Smith,
J. E. K. (1988). Optimality in human motor performance: Ideal
control of rapid aimed movements. Psychological Review, 95,
340–370.
Meyer, D. E., Smith, J. E. K., & Wright, C. E. (1982). Models for the
speed and accuracy of aimed movements. Psychological Review,
89,449–482.


Monsell, S. (1986). Programming of complex sequences: Evidence
from the timing of rapid speech and other productions. In
H. Heuer & C. Fromm (Eds.), Generation and modulation of
action patterns(pp. 72–86). Berlin, Germany: Springer.
Müller, H. (2001). Ausführungsvariabilität und Ergebniskonstanz
[Variability of execution and constancy of outcomes].
Lengerich: Pabst Science Publishers.
Müller, H., & Loosch, E. (1999). Functional variability and an equi-
final path of movement during targeted throwing. Journal of
Human Movement Studies, 36,103–126.


Näätänen, R., & Merisalo, A. (1977). Expectancy and preparation in
simple reaction time. In S. Dornic (Ed.), Attention and perfor-
mance VI(pp. 115–138) Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Nelson, W. L. (1983). Physical principles for economies of skilled
movements.Biological Cybernetics, 46,135–147.
Newell, K. M., & van Emmerik, R. E. A. (1989). The acquisition of
coordination: Preliminary analysis of learning to write. Human
Movement Science, 8,17–32.
Nielsen, T. I. (1963). Volition: A new experimental approach. Scan-
dinavian Journal of Psychology, 4,225–230.
Noble, M., Fitts, P. M., & Warren, C. E. (1955). The frequency
response of skilled subjects in a pursuit tracking task. Journal of
Experimental Psychology, 49,249–256.
Nougier, V., Bard, C., Fleury, M., Teasdale, N., Cole, J., Forget, R.,
Paillard, J., & Lamarre, Y. (1996). Control of single-joint move-
ments in deafferented patients: Evidence for amplitude coding
rather than position control. Experimental Brain Research, 109,
473–482.
Paillard, J. (1982). The contribution of peripheral and central vision
to visually guided reaching. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, &
R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.), Analysis of visual behavior
(pp. 367–385). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Peper, C. E. (1995). Tapping dynamics. Unpublished doctoral dis-
sertation, Free University Amsterdam.
Peper, C. E., & Beek, P. J. (1998). Are frequency-induced transitions
in rhythmic coordination mediated by a drop in amplitude?
Biological Cybernetics, 79,291–300.
Peper, L., Bootsma, R. J., Mestre, D. R., & Bakker, F. C. (1994).
Catching balls: How to get the hand to the right place at the right
time.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception
and Performance, 20,591–612.
Perenin, M. T., & Jeannerod, M. (1978). Visual function within the
hemianopic field following early cerebral hemidecortication in
man: I. Spatial localization. Neuropsychologia, 16,1–13.
Peters, M. (1981). Attentional asymmetries during concurrent
bimanual performance. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psy-
chology, 33A,95–103.
Pew, R.W. (1966). Acquisition of hierarchical control over the tem-
poral organization of skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology,
71,764–771.
Plamondon, R., & Alimi, A. M. (1997). Speed/accuracy trade-offs in
target-directed movements. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20,
279–349.
Polit, A., & Bizzi, E. (1979). Characteristics of motor programs
underlying arm movements in monkeys. Journal of Neurophysi-
ology, 42,183–194.
Poulton, E. C. (1957). On the stimulus and response in pursuit track-
ing.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 53,189–194.
Preilowski, B. (1972). Possible contribution of the anterior fore-
brain commissures to bilateral motor coordination. Neuropsy-
chologia, 10,267–277.
Free download pdf