Handbook of Psychology, Volume 4: Experimental Psychology

(Axel Boer) #1
References 353

Shadmehr, R., & Moussavi, Z. M. K. (2000). Spatial generalization
from learning dynamics of reaching movements. Journal of
Neuroscience, 20,7807–7815.
Shadmehr, R., & Mussa-Ivaldi, F. A. (1994). Adaptive representa-
tion of dynamics during learning of a motor task. Journal of
Neuroscience, 14,3208–3224.
Sherwood, D. E. (1991). Distance and location assimilation in rapid
bimanual movement. Research Quarterly for Exercise and
Sport, 62,302–308.
Smeets, J. B. J., & Brenner, E. (1995). Perception and action are
based on the same visual information: Distinction between posi-
tion and velocity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance, 21,19–31.
Smeets, J. B. J., Erkelens, C. J., & Denier van der Gon, J. J. (1995).
Perturbations of fast goal-directed arm movements: Different
behavior of early and late EMG responses. Journal of Motor
Behavior, 27,77–88.
Spijkers, W. (1993). Sehen und Handeln: Die Rolle visueller Infor-
mation bei zielgerichteten Bewegungen [Perception and action:
The role of visual information in aimed movements]. Aachen,
Germany: Shaker.
Spijkers, W., & Heuer, H. (1995). Structural constraints on the per-
formance of symmetrical bimanual movements with different
amplitudes.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 48A,
716–740.
Spijkers, W., Heuer, H., Kleinsorge, T., & Steglich, C. (2000). The
specification of movement amplitudes for the left and right hand:
Evidence for transient parametric coupling from overlapping-
task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human
Perception and Performance, 26,1091–1105.
Spijkers, W., Heuer, H., Kleinsorge, T., & van der Loo, H. (1997).
Preparation of bimanual movements with same and different am-
plitudes: Specification interference as revealed by reaction time.
Acta Psychologica, 96,207–227.
Spijkers, W., Tachmatzidis, K., Debus, G., Fischer, M., & Kausche,
I. (1994). Temporal coordination of alternative and simultaneous
aiming movements of constrained timing structure. Psychologi-
cal Research, 57,20–29.
Steglich, C., Heuer, H., Spijkers, W., & Kleinsorge, T. (1999).
Bimanual coupling during the specification of isometric forces.
Experimental Brain Research, 129,302–316.
Stelmach, G. E. (1982). Motor control and motor learning:
The closed-loop perspective. In J. A. S. Kelso (Ed.),Human motor
behavior: An introduction(pp. 93–115). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Stelmach, G. E., Kelso, J. A. S., & Wallace, S. A. (1975). Preselec-
tion in short-term motor memory. Journal of Experimental Psy-
chology: Human Learning and Memory, 1,745–755.
Stimpel, E. (1933). Der Wurf [The throw]. Neue Psychologische
Studien, 9,105–138.
Stratton, G. M. (1896). Some preliminary experiments in vision
without inversion of the retinal image. Psychological Review, 3,
611–617.


Stratton, G. M. (1897a). Upright vision and the retinal image. Psy-
chological Review, 4,182–187.
Stratton, G. M. (1897b). Vision without inversion of the retinal
image.Psychological Review, 4,341–360, 463–481.
Stucchi, N., & Viviani, P. (1993). Cerebral dominance and asyn-
chrony between bimanual two-dimensional movements. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Perfor-
mance, 19,1200–1220.
Summers, J. J., Rosenbaum, D. A., Burns, B. D., & Ford, S. K.
(1993). Production of polyrhythms. Journal of Experimen-
tal Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 19,416–
428.
Swinnen, S. P., Jardin, K., & Meulenbroek, R. (1996). Between-
limb asynchronies during bimanual coordination: Effects of
manual dominance and attentional cueing. Neuropsychologica,
34,1203–1213.
Taub, E., Goldberg, I. A., & Taub, P. (1975). Deafferentation in
monkeys: Pointing at a target without visual feedback. Experi-
mental Neurology, 46,178–186.
Teasdale, N., Forget, R., Bard, C., Paillard, J., Fleury, M., &
Lamarre, Y. (1993). The role of proprioceptive information for
the production of isometric forces and for handwriting tasks.
Acta Psychologica,82,179–191.
Tendick, F., Jennings, R. W., Tharp, G., & Stark, L. (1993). Sensing
and manipulation problems in endoscopic surgery: experiment,
analysis, and observation. Presence, 2,66–81.
Teulings, H.-L. (1996). Handwriting movement control. In H.
Heuer & S. W. Keele (Eds.), Handbook of perception and ac-
tion: Vol. 2. Motor skills(pp. 561–613). London: Academic
Press.
Todor, J. I., & Lazarus, J. C. (1986). Exertion level and the intensity
of associated movements. Developmental Medicine & Child
Neurology, 28,205–212.
Tresilian, J. R. (1999). Analysis of recent empirical challenges to an
account of interceptive timing. Perception & Psychophysics, 61,
515–528.
Tuller, B., & Kelso, J. A. S. (1989). Environmentally-specified pat-
terns of movement coordination in normal and split-brain sub-
jects.Experimental Brain Research, 75,306–316.
Tyldesley, D. A., & Whiting, H. T. A. (1975). Operational timing.
Journal of Human Movement Studies, 1,172–177.
Ungerleider, L. G., & Mishkin, M. (1982). Two cortical visual sys-
tems. In D. J. Ingle, M. A. Goodale, & R. J. W. Mansfield (Eds.),
Analysis of visual behavior(pp. 549–586). Cambridge, MA: MIT
Press.
Uno, Y., Kawato, M., & Suzuki, R. (1989). Formation and control of
optimal trajectory in human multijoint arm movement: Mini-
mum torque-change model. Biological Cybernetics, 61,89–101.
Vindras, P., & Viviani, P. (1998). Frames of reference and control
parameters in visuo manual pointint. Journal of Experimental
Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24,569–591.
Free download pdf