Teaching Critical Thinking in Psychology: A Handbook of Best Practices

(ff) #1

Laird R. O. Edman


38


Developmental Theories

Perry (1970) pioneered the developmental approach and constructed his influential


scheme of epistemological development through questionnaires and interviews with


Harvard undergraduates in the 1950s and 1960s. Perry asserted that undergraduate stu-


dents’ personal epistemology could be classified along a continuum of four stages or clus-


ters which could be further subdivided into nine sequential phases. Students in the more


naïve stages view knowledge as concrete, absolute, and handed down from authorities


(e.g., “We know the universe is 13.7 billion years old because experts say so, and learning


means memorizing what the experts say”), whereas students in the more developed stages


view knowledge claims as tentative and in need of justification (e.g., “The current state


of evidence in several fields indicates the universe is about 13.7 billion years old, but


new evidence might be found to challenge that theory. We should accept whatever the best


evidence says for now”). This important early theory of epistemological development has


been revised and developed by a number of researchers using different methods with dif-


ferent populations (Baxter Magolda, 1992; Belenky, Clinchy, Goldberger, & Tarule, 1986;


Kuhn, 1999; Kuhn & Weinstock, 2002). While these other theories have important dif-


ferences in approach, definitions of epistemology, and conclusions, the fundamental out-


line of Perry’s work remains within all of the subsequent epistemological development


theories (Hofer & Pintrich, 1997).


Reflective Thinking Model

The most thoroughly researched and perhaps most rigorously empirical developmental


theory of personal epistemology is that of King and Kitchener (1994, 2002, 2004). King


and Kitchener’s reflective judgment model focuses on justification for belief as the key


component to discerning someone’s epistemological stance. In this approach, students’


epistemological development is best understood through their explanations for their


answers to difficult issues rather than the actual content of their answers—examining why


they believe what they believe, rather than examining what they believe.


King and Kitchener have developed and tested their model using their Reflective


Judgment Interview, an hour-long interview through which they observe participants’


answers and justifications for their answers to ill-structured problems (problems which


cannot be defined with a high degree of completeness nor solved with certainty, e.g.,


“How can we reconcile conflicting accounts of the origin and diversity of species on the


planet?”). The protocols of over 8,000 participants over 25 years in dozens of studies


across ages, gender, educational levels, backgrounds, and ethnicities have revealed strong


evidence for striking differences in people’s underlying assumptions about knowledge, dif-


ferences that are related to how people make and justify their judgments and which change


in a developmental sequence that is related to both age and education (King & Kitchener,


2002, 2004).


Prereflective thinking. King and Kitchener (1994) identified seven stages of reflective


thinking organized into three different levels. Stages 1–3, called prereflective thinking,

Free download pdf