Consciousness

(Tuis.) #1

  • seCtIon FIVe: BoRDeRLAnDs
    two dimensions: irrationality and ability to hal-
    lucinate. By plotting a person’s position in this
    space, he imagined just three major clusters
    corresponding to the states of dreaming, lucid
    dreaming (Chapter  15), and ordinary conscious-
    ness. All other positions in the space cannot be
    occupied, or are unstable. So you may briefly
    hover between waking and dreaming, but this
    state is unstable and rapidly gives way to one
    of the others. For this reason, Tart refers to the
    occupied areas as ‘discrete states of conscious-
    ness’. To move out of such a region, you have
    to cross a ‘forbidden zone’ where you cannot
    stably function or have experiences, until you
    reach a discretely different experiential space. In
    other words, you can be here or there, but not in
    between. Just how many states are discrete like
    this we do not know: Tart’s scheme was only a
    limited and quite informal way of starting to map
    states into a space.
    A second and more systematic two-dimensional space is described by Steven
    Laureys (2005). His dimensions are completely different: level of arousal and
    awareness of environment and self. Arousal refers to physiological wakefulness
    or the ‘level’ of consciousness and is dependent on the brainstem arousal sys-
    tem. Awareness of environment and self refers to the ‘content’ of consciousness
    and requires a functionally integrated cortex with its subcortical loops. A simple
    diagram shows that for most states level and content are positively correlated.
    As Laureys puts it, ‘You need to be awake in order to be aware (REM sleep being
    a notable exception)’ (2009, p. 58). Other exceptions discussed by Laureys are the
    vegetative state, sleepwalking, and some kinds of seizure, all of which involve
    some wakefulness with no apparent awareness.
    The AIM model is a three-dimensional map developed by American psychiatrist
    and sleep researcher Allan Hobson (1999), and named after its dimensions. Acti-
    vation energy is similar to arousal and can be measured, for example, using EEG.
    Input source can vary between entirely ‘external’ or entirely ‘internal’ sources of
    information. Mode is the ratio of amines to cholines. During waking, the amine
    neurotransmitters and neuromodulators, including noradrenaline and serotonin,
    predominate and are essential for rational thought, volition, and directing atten-
    tion. During REM sleep acetylcholine takes over and thinking becomes delusional,
    irrational, and unreflective. The ratio of these two is Hobson’s mode.
    States can now be positioned in what Hobson calls ‘brain–mind space’ by measuring
    them along these three dimensions. He stresses that it is an entirely artificial model,
    yet is based on specific data and recognises the continuously changing nature of
    brain–mind states. Unlike in Tart’s early model, any state of the brain–mind can be
    positioned within it, and any area in the space can in theory be occupied.
    Other dimensional models derive from studies of specific corners of the vast
    consciousness state–space. A  survey of ‘meditation depth’ amongst 300 yoga,
    Buddhist, and TM meditators resulted in another three dimensions: mystical


HIGH

LOW HIGH


LOW

(STAGE1–STATREMDE2REAMING)
(LUCID DREAMING)STATE3

(ORDINARY CONSCIOUSNESS)STATE1

DIMENSION2(RAT IONALITY)

DIMENSION 1
(ABILITYT

O HALLUCINATE)

FIGURE 13.3 • Tart’s plot of three discrete
altered states in a space with
two dimensions: irrationality and
ability to hallucinate (after Tart,
1975, Fig. 5.1).

Free download pdf