30 | New Scientist | 7 March 2020
Book
Desert Navigator:
The journey of an ant
Rüdiger Wehner
Harvard University Press
“CATAGLYPHIS is only an ant, but
truly what an ant,” writes Rüdiger
Wehner in the prologue to Desert
Navigator. A chapter into his new
book, I also came under the spell
of this long-legged “racehorse
of the insect world” and the
staggering navigational skills
that emerge from the 500,000
neurons in its brain, which weighs
less than a tenth of a milligram.
As a young biologist, Wehner
fell for them when he visited a
Tunisian salt pan and saw one
pick up a dead insect and sprint
100 metres across the featureless
desert to its inconspicuous nest
hole. How did the ant know where
to go? Fifty years later, thanks to
Wehner, now director emeritus
of the Institute of Zoology at the
University of Zurich, Switzerland,
and the researchers he inspired,
we have many of the answers, laid
out in this grand book.
As Wehner realised early on ,
Cataglyphis is a “model organism”
for studying animal navigation.
The ants run about on flat, open
surfaces, so it is easy to track them
and explore their talents. The
secrets revealed in five decades
of research are extraordinary.
One of the first surprises is that
these ants can do what human
navigators call “dead reckoning”:
keeping a continuous tally of both
direction and distance covered
so that they can always compute
the direction of home.
As their compass, the ants use
This ant has superpowers
The amazing desert ant runs at breakneck speeds and uses the sun, wind and
magnetic fields to navigate featureless deserts, discovers Alun Anderson
the polarisation pattern of the
sky, as well as Earth’s magnetic
field and the desert wind.
Gauging distance is done with a
“pedometer” – the ant counts its
steps – and by sensing the “optic
flow”, or relative motion, of the
ground beneath it as it runs.
If the desert contains landmarks,
such as rocks, the ants are superb
at using them for navigation too.
When an ant leaves the nest,
Wehner describes how it faces
back towards the entrance and
pauses to take “snapshots” of its
surroundings. Later, it compares
those snapshots with the current
view to spot the way home.
Wehner explains many more
fascinating ant skills before he
moves on to his pioneering
studies on the harder question
of how the ant integrates all that
Views Culture
CO
UR
TE
SY
OF
RÜ
DIG
ER
W
EH
NE
R
you might do on a stroll from a
hotel in an unfamiliar city. You
may not know where you are, but
you can find your way home by
remembering things like: “I
turned left at a big supermarket.”
Desert Navigator is written in
a concise and serious style. You
will need to give time to its many
explanatory diagrams, but the
rewards are insights into “the
process of thought that leads to
the solution”, and the ingenuity
of the experiments that Wehner
and others have devised to work
out how the ants do their thing.
For example, to show that the
insects were counting steps to
estimate distance, tiny stilts were
glued to their legs to lengthen
their stride. Sure enough, they
changed their distance estimates.
And to show that the ants really
sense optic flow, a runway was
built in the desert with a moving
pattern of stripes beneath it.
That, too, altered their perception
of how far they had run.
You will end the book as a fan
of Cataglyphis, for the creature is
more than just a navigator. Early in
life, the ants nurse the brood, then
become “excavators”, and finally,
after initial timid trips, they sprint
off from the cool of the nest into
the ferocious desert heat to forage.
Outside, their achievements
are amazing. They can move fast,
covering up to 110 body lengths
a second; in human terms, about
600 kilometres per hour. They
must dodge danger everywhere.
Robber flies attack from the air,
jumping spiders from the surface
and tiger beetles from below
ground. A forager is usually dead
within five to seven days, even
though the ants live for months
in a lab. But food must be found.
What a life and, yes, what an ant. ❚
Alun Anderson is an editor emeritus
of New Scientist
information with its memory
to decide where to go. This is
the frontier where field studies
meet computational modelling,
robotics and neuroscience.
So far, research seems to settle
one long-running debate: the ants
don’t form a mental “map” of their
surroundings as many researchers
have argued. Although they are
superb navigators, they keep
things simple. They never actually
know where they are but only
where to go. If that sounds
confusing, it is a little like what
Cataglyphis takes
“snapshots” of landmarks
to help it locate its nest
“ These insects do what
we call dead reckoning,
tallying direction and
distance so they always
know the way home”