Evolution And History

(Marvins-Underground-K-12) #1

56 CHAPTER 3 | Living Primates


of the protected preserve. These gorillas
spend close to 90 percent of their time
outside the national park, in and around
human-inhabited areas and farms. These
behavioral changes have many costs to
the gorillas, such as increased contact
with humans and human waste, conflict
with farmers that could result in injury,
increased exposure to hunting as these
areas are mostly open fields, and in-
creased risk of disease transmission.
Another effect on behavior may be an
artificial increase in group size. For ex-
ample, a group of some forty-four animals
now exist in the Virunga Mountains [along
the border of Rwanda, the Democratic
Republic of Congo, and Uganda] where
the average group size is usually ten
individuals. Furthermore, it is thought
that, due to their fear of humans, nonha-
bituated adult male gorillas that would
normally challenge other dominant males
are either deterred from presenting a

challenge or are less successful in their
challenge against habituated groups.
Perhaps the biggest threat to habitu-
ated great apes is disease. There are
over nineteen viruses and eighteen para-
sites that are known to infect both great
apes and humans. These diseases have
been responsible for be tween sixty-three
and eighty-seven ape deaths in habitu-
ated groups (both research and tourist
groups) in the Virungas, Bwindi, Mahale,
Tai, and Gombe.c As for the gorillas in
Bwindi, it has been shown that the prev-
alence of parasites such as Crytopspo-
ridium and Giardia are most prevalent
in habituated groups living near humans
along the border of the park.
In highlighting the costs of habitu-
ation in field primatology, as a great
ape primatologist, I know full well the
benefits that have come out of this
process. Weighing these costs and ben-
efits as a biological anthropologist, I
wonder if primatological field studies on
endangered great apes for the sake of

understanding humans is still a viable
option. Perhaps primatologists should
study apes only when it directly benefits
the welfare and conservation of the
study animals, rather than our interest
or curiosity in learning more ourselves.
Ethical considerations are crucial as the
numbers of great apes in the wild con-
tinue to dwindle. Habituation may not
be an ape’s salvation.

aInternational Union for Conservation of
Nature and Natural Resources (IUCN).
(2000).
bBlom, A., et al. (2001). A survey of the
apes in the Dzanga-Ndoki National Park,
Central African Republic. African Jour-
nal of Ecology 39 , 98–105.
cButynski, T. M. (2001). Africa’s great
apes. In B. Beck et al. (Eds.), Great apes
and humans: The ethics of co-existence
(pp. 3–56). Washington, DC: Smithso-
nian Institution Press.

CONTINUED

Primates as Mammals


Biologists classify humans within the primate order, a
subgroup of the class Mammalia. The other primates
include lemurs, lorises, tarsiers, monkeys, and apes.
Humans— together with chimpanzees, bonobos, goril-
las, orangutans, gibbons, and siamangs—form the hom-
inoids, colloquially known as apes, a superfamily within
the primate order. Biologically speaking, as hominoids,
humans are apes.
The primates are only one of several different kinds
of mammals, such as rodents, carnivores, and ungulates
(hoofed mammals). Primates, like other mammals, are in-
telligent animals, having more in the way of brains than
reptiles or other kinds of vertebrates. This increased brain
power, along with the mammalian pattern of growth and
development, forms the biological basis of the flexible
behavior patterns typical of mammals. In most species,
the young are born live, the egg being retained within
the womb of the female until the embryo achieves an ad-
vanced state of growth.
Once born, the young receive milk from their mother’s
mammary glands, the physical feature from which the class
Mammalia gets its name. During this period of infant de-
pendency, young mammals learn many of the things they
will need for survival as adults. Primates in general, and
apes in particular, have a very long period of infant and
childhood dependency in which the young learn the ways
of their social group. Thus, mammalian primate biology is
central to primate behavioral patterns.


Relative to other members of the animal kingdom,
mammals are highly active. This activity is made possible
by a relatively constant body temperature, an efficient re-
spiratory system featuring a separation between the nasal
and mouth cavities (allowing them to breathe while they
eat), a diaphragm to assist in drawing in and letting out
breath, and a four-chambered heart that prevents mixing
of oxygenated and deoxygenated blood.
Mammals possess a skeleton in which the limbs are
positioned beneath the body, rather than out at the sides.
This arrangement allows for direct support of the body
and easy flexible movement. The bones of the limbs
have joints constructed to permit growth in the young
while simultaneously providing strong, hard joint sur-
faces that will stand up to the stresses of sustained activ-
ity. Mammals stop growing when they reach adulthood,
while reptiles continue to grow throughout their lives.
Mammals and reptiles also differ in terms of their
teeth. Reptiles possess identical, pointed, peglike teeth
while mammals have teeth specialized for particular pur-
poses: incisors for nipping, gnawing, and cutting; canines
for ripping, tearing, killing, and fighting; premolars that
may either slice and tear or crush and grind (depending on
the kind of animal); and molars for crushing and grinding
(Figure 3.2). This enables mammals to eat a wide variety
of food—an advantage to them, since they require more
food than reptiles to sustain their high activity level. But
they pay a price: Reptiles have unlimited tooth replace-
ment throughout their lives, whereas mammals are limited
to two sets. The first set serves the immature animal and is
Free download pdf