Social Research Methods: Qualitative and Quantitative Approaches

(Brent) #1
FIELD RESEARCH AND FOCUS GROUP RESEARCH


  1. See Geertz (1973, 1979) on “thick description.” Also
    see Denzin (1989:159–160) for additional discussion.

  2. For more on ethnomethodology, see Cicourel (1964),
    Denzin (1970), Leiter (1980), Mehan and Wood (1975),
    and Turner (1974). Also see Emerson (1981:357–359)
    and Lester and Hadden (1980) on the relationship between
    field research and ethnomethodology. Garfinkel (1974a)
    discussed the origins of the term ethnomethodology.

  3. The misunderstandings of people resulting from the
    disjuncture of different cultures is a common theme.

  4. For a general discussion of field research and natu-
    ralism, see Adler and Adler (1994), Georges and Jones
    (1980), Holy (1984), and Pearsall (1970). For discus-
    sions of contrasting types of field research, see Clammer
    (1984), Gonor (1977), Holstein and Gubrium (1994),
    Morse (1994), Schwandt (1994), and Strauss and Corbin
    (1994).

  5. See Georges and Jones (1980:21–42) and Lofland
    et al. (2006:11–15).

  6. Johnson (1975:65–66) has discussed defocusing.

  7. See Lofland (1976:13–23) and Shaffir et al. (1980:
    18–20) on feeling marginal.

  8. See Adler and Adler (1987:67–78).

  9. See Hammersley and Atkinson (1983:42–45) and
    Lofland et al. (2006:17–32).

  10. Jewish researchers have studied Christians (Kleinman,
    1980), Whites have studied African Americans (Liebow,
    1967), and adult researchers have become intimate with
    youngsters (Fine, 1987; Fine and Glassner, 1979; Thorne
    and Luria, 1986). Also see Eichler (1988), Hunt (1989),
    and Wax (1979) on the role of race, gender, and age in
    field research.

  11. See Douglas and Rasmussen (1977) and Yancey and
    Rainwater (1970).

  12. For more on gatekeepers and access, see Beck
    (1970:11–29), Bogdan and Taylor (1975:30–32), Corra
    and Willer (2002), and Wax (1971:367).

  13. Adapted from Gray (1980:311). See also Hicks
    (1984) and Schatzman and Strauss (1973:58–63).

  14. For discussions of ascribed status (and, in particular,
    gender) in field research, see Adler and Adler (1987),
    Ardener (1984), Ayella (1993), Denzin (1989:116–118),
    Douglas (1976), Easterday et al. (1982), Edwards (1993),
    Lofland et al. (2006:22-24), and Van Maanen (1982).

  15. Roy (1970) argued for the “Ernie Pyle” role based
    on his study of union organizing in the southern United
    States. In this role, named after a World War II war cor-
    respondent, the researcher “goes with the troops” as a
    type of participant as observer. Trice (1970) discussed
    the advantages of an outsider role. Schwartz and Schwartz
    (1969) discussed various roles.
    20. See Douglas (1976), Emerson (1981:367–368), and
    Johnson (1975:124–129) on being patient, polite, and
    considerate.
    21. Negotiation in the field is discussed in Gans (1982),
    Johnson (1975:58–59, 76–77), and Schatzman and
    Strauss (1973:22–23).
    22. On entering and gaining access to field sites with
    deviant groups, see Becker (1970a:31–38), Hammersley
    and Atkinson (1983:54–76), Lofland et al. (2006:30–47),
    and West (1980). Elite access is discussed by Hoffman
    (1980).
    23. See Lofland et al. (2006:22-25).
    24. For discussion of “normalizing,” see Gans (1982:
    57–59), Georges and Jones (1980:43–164), Hammersley
    and Atkinson (1983:70–76), Harkens and Warren
    (1993), Johnson (1975), and Wax (1971). Mann (1970)
    discussed how to teach members about a researcher’s
    role.
    25. For more on roles in field settings, see Barnes
    (1970:241–244), Emerson (1981:364), Hammersley and
    Atkinson (1983:88–104), Warren and Rasmussen (1977),
    and Wax (1979). On dress, see Bogdan and Taylor (1975:
    45) and Douglas (1976).
    26. See Lofland (1976) and Lofland et al. (2006) on
    focusing. Spradley (1979b:100–111) also provides a
    helpful discussion.
    27. See Denzin (1989:71–73, 86–92), Glaser and
    Strauss (1967), Hammersley and Atkinson (1983: 45–53),
    Honigmann (1982), and Weiss (1994:25–29) on sampling
    in field research.
    28. See Gurevitch (1988), Hammersley and Atkinson
    (1983), and Schatzman and Strauss (1973:53) on
    “strangeness” in field research.
    29. See Gans (1982), Goward (1984b), and Van Maanen
    (1983b:282–286).
    30. See Douglas (1976:216) and Corsino (1987).
    31. See Warren and Rasmussen (1977) for a discussion
    of cross-gender tension.
    32. See Wax (1971:13).
    33. Also see Adler and Adler (1987:40–42), Bogdan and
    Taylor (1975:35–37), Douglas (1976), and Gray (1980:
    321).
    34. See Bogdan and Taylor (1975:50–51), Lofland et al.
    (2006:57–60), Shupe and Bromley (1980), and Wax
    (1971).
    35. See Johnson (1975:105–108).
    36. The acceptable incompetent or learner role is dis-
    cussed in Bogdan and Taylor (1975:46), Douglas (1976),
    Hammersley and Atkinson (1983:92–94), Lofland et al.
    (2006:55-57), and Schatman and Strauss (1973:25).
    37. See Strauss (1987:10–11).

Free download pdf