Handbook of Psychology, Volume 4: Experimental Psychology

(Axel Boer) #1

392 Conditioning and Learning


Fantino, E., Preston, R. A., & Dunn, R. (1993). Delay reduction:
Current status. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,
60,159–169.
Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement.
New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Foree, D. D., & LoLordo, V. M. (1975). Stimulus-reinforcer inter-
actions in the pigeon: The role of electric shock and the avoid-
ance contingency. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal
Behavior Processes, 1,39–46.
Gallistel, C. R., & Gibbon, J. (2000). Time, rate and conditioning.
Psychological Review, 107,219–275.
Garcia, J., & Koelling, R. A. (1966). Relation of cue to consequence
in avoidance learning. Psychonomic Science, 4,123–124.
Garcia, J., Ervin, F. R., & Koelling, R. A. (1966). Learning with
prolonged delay of reinforcement. Psychonomic Science, 5,
121–122.
Garcia, J., Lasiter, P. S., Bermudez-Rattoni, F., & Deems, D. A.
(1985). A general theory of aversion learning. Annals of the New
York Academy of Sciences, 443,8–21.
Gibbon, J. (1995). Dynamics of time matching: Arousal makes
better seem worse. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 2,208–215.
Gibbon, J., & Balsam, P. (1981). Spreading association in time. In
C. M. Locurto, H. S. Terrace, & J. Gibbon (Eds.), Autoshaping
and conditioning theory(pp. 219–253). New York: Academic
Press.
Gibbon, J., Baldock, M. D., Locurto, C., Gold, L., & Terrace, H. S.
(1977). Trial and intertrial durations in autoshaping. Journal
of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 3,
264–284.
Grace, R. C. (1994). A contextual model of concurrent-chains
choice.Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 61,
113–129.
Grace, R. C. (1996). Choice between fixed and variable delays to
reinforcement in the adjusting-delay procedure and concurrent
chains.Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior
Processes, 22,362–383.
Grace, R. C. (1999). The matching law and amount-dependent ex-
ponential discounting as accounts of self-control choice. Journal
of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 71,27–44.
Green, L., & Freed, D. E. (1998). Behavioral economics. In
W. O’Donohue (Ed.), Learning and behavior therapy(pp. 274–
300). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.
Green, L., & Snyderman, M. (1980). Choice between rewards dif-
fering in amount and delay: Toward a choice model of self
control.Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 34,
135–147.
Groves, P. M., & Thompson, R. F. (1970). Habituation: A dual-
process theory. Psychological Review, 77,419–450.
Guthrie, E. R. (1935).The psychology of learning.New York: Harper.
Guthrie, E. R. (1938). The psychology of human conflict.New York:
Harper.


Guttman, N., & Kalish, H. I. (1956). Discriminability and stimulus
generalization.Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51,79–88.
Gwinn, G. T. (1949). The effects of punishment on acts motivated
by fear. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 39,260–269.
Hall, G. (1991). Perceptual and associative learning.Oxford, UK:
Oxford University Press.
Hall, G. (1996). Learning about associatively activated stimulus
representations: Implications for acquired equivalence in per-
ceptual learning. Animal Learning & Behavior, 24,233–255.
Hallam, S. C., Grahame, N. J., Harris, K., & Miller, R. R. (1992).
Associative structures underlying enhanced negative summation
following operational extinction of a Pavlovian inhibitor. Learn-
ing and Motivation, 23,43–62.
Hammond, L. J. (1980). The effect of contingency upon the appeti-
tive conditioning of free-operant behavior. Journal of the Exper-
imental Analysis of Behavior, 34,297–304.
Hearst, E. (1988). Fundamentals of learning and conditioning. In
R. C. Atkinson, R. J. Herrnstein, G. Lindzey, & R. D. Luce
(Eds.),Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology: Vol. 2.
Learning and cognition(pp. 3–109). New York: Wiley.
Herrnstein, R. J. (1961). Relative and absolute strength of response
as a function of frequency of reinforcement. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4,267–272.
Herrnstein, R. J. (1964). Secondary reinforcement and rate of pri-
mary reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
Behavior, 7,27–36.
Herrnstein, R. J. (1970). On the law of effect. Journal of the Exper-
imental Analysis of Behavior, 13,243–266.
Herrnstein, R. J., & Heyman, G. M. (1979). Is matching compatible
with reinforcement maxmization on concurrent variable interval,
variable ratio? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,
31,209–223.
Herrnstein, R. J., & Prelec, D. (1992). A theory of addiction. In
G. Loewenstein & J. Elster (Eds.), Choice over time(pp. 331–
360). New York: Russell Sage.
Herrnstein, R. J., & Vaughan, W. (1980). Melioration and behavior
allocation. In J. E. R. Staddon (Ed.), Limits to action(pp. 143–
176). New York: Academic Press.
Heth, C. D. (1976). Simultaneous and backward fear conditioning
as a function of number of CS-UCS pairings.Journal of Experi-
mental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 2,117–129.
Heyman, G. M. (1996). Resolving the contradictions of addiction.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 19,561–610.
Heyman, G. M., & Luce, R. D. (1979). Operant matching is not a
logical consequence of maximizing reinforcement rate. Animal
Learning & Behavior, 7,133–140.
Heyman, G. M., & Tanz, L. (1995). How to teach a pigeon to maxi-
mize overall reinforcement rate. Journal of the Experimental
Analysis of Behavior, 64,277–297.
Hinson, J. M., & Staddon, J. E. R. (1983). Hill-climbing by pigeons.
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 39,25–47.
Free download pdf