Handbook of Psychology, Volume 4: Experimental Psychology

(Axel Boer) #1

396 Conditioning and Learning


St. Claire-Smith, R. (1979). The overshadowing and blocking of
punishment.Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31,
51–61.
Saint Paul, U. v. (1982). Do geese use path integration for walking
home? In F. Papi & H. G. Wallraff (Eds.), Avian navigation
(pp. 298–307). New York: Springer.
Savastano, H. I., & Fantino, E. (1994). Human choice in concurrent
ratio-interval schedules of reinforcement. Journal of the Experi-
mental Analysis of Behavior, 61,453–463.
Savastano, H. I., & Miller, R. R. (1998). Time as content in Pavlovian
conditioning.Behavioural Processes, 44,147–162.
Schaal, D. W., & Branch, M. N. (1988). Responding of pigeons
under variable-interval schedules of unsignaled, briefly signaled,
and completely signaled delays to reinforcement. Journal of the
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 50,33–54.
Schmajuk, N. A., Lamoureux, J. A., & Holland, P. C. (1998). Occa-
sion setting: A neural network approach. Psychological Review,
105,3–32.
Schneider, B. (1969). A two-state analysis of fixed-interval respond-
ing in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of
Behavior, 12,667–687.
Shanks, D. R. (1994). Human associative learning. In N. J.
Mackintosh (Ed.), Animal learning and cognition(pp. 335–374).
San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
Shettleworth, S. J. (1998). Cognition, evolution, and behavior.New
York: Oxford.
Shimp, C. P. (1966). Probabilistically reinforced choice behavior in
pigeons.Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9,
433–455.
Shimp, C. P. (1969). Optimum behavior in free-operant experi-
ments.Psychological Review, 76,97–112.
Sidman, M. (1960). Tactics of scientific research.New York: Basic
Books.
Siegel, S. (1989). Pharmacological conditioning and drug effects. In
A. J. Goudie & M. W. Emmet-Oglesby (Eds.), Psychoactive
drugs: Tolerance and sensitization(pp. 115–185). Clifton, NJ:
Humana Press.
Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms.New York:
Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Skinner, B. F. (1969). Contingencies of reinforcement: A theoretical
analysis.New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Slamecka, N. J., & Ceraso, J. (1960). Retroactive and proactive in-
hibition of verbal learning. Psychological Bulletin, 57,449–475.
Spence, K. W. (1936). The nature of discrimination learning in ani-
mals.Psychological Review, 43,427–449.
Staddon, J. E. R. (1977). On Herrnstein’s equation and related
forms.Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 28,
163–170.
Staddon, J. E. R. (1979). Operant behavior as adaptation to con-
straint.Journal of Experimental Psychology: General,108,
48–67.


Staddon, J. E. R., & Simmelhag, V. L. (1970). The “superstition” ex-
periment: A reexamination of its implications for the principles
of adaptive behavior. Psychological Review, 78,3–43.
Sulzer-Azaroff, B., & Mayer, R. G. (1991). Behavior analysis for
lasting change.Ft. Worth, TX: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.
Sutton, R. S., & Barto, A. G. (1981). Toward a modern theory of
adaptive networks: Expectation and prediction. Psychological
Review, 88,135–170.
Thompson, R. F., & Spencer, W. A. (1966). Habituation: A model
phenomenon for the study of neuronal substrates of behavior.
Psychological Review, 73,16–43.
Thorndike, E. L. (1898). Animal intelligence: An experimental
study of the associative processes in animals. Psychological
Monographs, 2,No. 8.
Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Animal intelligence: Experimental studies.
New York: Macmillan.
Thorndike, E. L. (1932). Fundamentals of learning.New York:
Columbia University.
Thorndike, E. L. (1949). Selected writings from a connectionist’s
psychology.East Norwalk, CT: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Timberlake, W. (1984). Behavior regulation and learned perfor-
mance: Some misapprehensions and disagreements. Journal of
the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 41,355–375.
Timberlake, W., & Allison, J. (1974). Response deprivation: An
empirical approach to instrumental performance. Psychological
Review, 81,146–164.
Timberlake, W., & Lucas, G. A. (1989). Behavior systems and learn-
ing: From misbehavior to general principles. In S. B. Klein &
R. R. Mowrer (Eds.), Contemporary learning theories: Instru-
mental conditioning theory and the impact of biological con-
straints in learning(pp. 237–275). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Timberlake, W., & Lucas, G. A. (1991). Periodic water, interwater
interval, and adjunctive behavior in a 24-hour multiresponse
environment.Animal Learning & Behavior, 19,369–380.
Tolman, E. C. (1932). Purposive behavior in animals and men.
London: Century/Random House.
Tolman, E. C., & Honzik, C. H. (1930). Introduction and removal of
reward, and maze performance in rats. University of California
Publications in Psychology, 4,257–275.
Van Hamme, L. J., & Wasserman, E. A. (1994). Cue competition in
causality judgments: The role of nonpresentation of compound
stimulus elements. Learning and Motivation, 25,127–151.
Vaughan, W. (1981). Melioration, matching, and maximization.
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 36,141–149.
Wagner, A. R. (1978). Expectancies and the priming of STM. In S. H.
Hulse, H. Fowler, & W. K. Honig (Eds.),Cognitive processes in
animal behavior(pp. 177–209). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wagner, A. R. (1981). SOP: A model of automatic memory process-
ing in animal behavior. In N. E. Spear & R. R. Miller (Eds.),
Information processing in animals: Memory mechanisms
(pp. 5–47). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Free download pdf