Teaching Critical Thinking in Psychology: A Handbook of Best Practices

(ff) #1

131


The Repertory Grid


(Mayo, 2004a). Dividing the course into three instructional units, I generated a list of


bipolar constructs on which students rated the positions of 24 preselected contributors


(8 per unit) to philosophical/prescientific psychology (e.g., Aristotle, René Descartes, John


Locke, and Immanuel Kant); the early years of scientific psychology (e.g., Wilhelm Wundt,


William James, Sigmund Freud, and John B. Watson); and the chronological development


of psychology’s principal specialty areas over the past century (e.g., Kurt Lewin, Gordon


Allport, Noam Chomsky, and Carl Rogers). Using Lundin’s (1996) text as a frame of


reference, I selected the first six constructs to represent deep-rooted issues in the intellectual


history of psychology: mind–body, nature–nurture, subjectivism–objectivism, holism–


elementalism, free will–determinism, and utility–purity. In contrast, I designed the final


two constructs (verity–falsity and major contribution–minor contribution) in an attempt


to ascertain students’ subjective views of each contributor’s work. In rating the degree of


perceived truth embodied in each contributor’s perspective, I asked students to consider


the sociohistorical factors in which each contribution was made. In the case of major


versus minor contribution, I asked students to assess each contributor’s legacy to the


historical evolution of psychology.


I asked students to rate the intellectual, philosophical, or theoretical stance of each


contributor as take-home, paper-and-pencil assignments in each unit of the course.


I employed a series of 11-point rating scales to allow students a wide range of variability in


their rating choices. I required that students work independently in completing each


assignment. In recording their construct-specific ratings on each grid, I asked students to


print an X on the appropriate line within each rating continuum—somewhere between


lines 1 and 11—that most closely approximates the perspective of each contributor.


Different from the more traditional application of the RGT as I described earlier in my


life span development course, I also asked students to provide written justification for each


of their construct ratings. In offering supporting rationale for their ratings, students


became increasingly aware of the value of evidence-based conclusions over unsubstantiated


opinions. Moreover, I allotted 30–45 mins for whole-class discussion immediately after


students completed each of the three rating-grid assignments. During these discussions,


students shared their analyses and critiqued their classmates’ expressed views.


Implications for the Undergraduate Psychology Curriculum

Altogether, the learning outcomes that I have observed in my own undergraduate


psychology classes (Mayo, 2004a, 2004b) suggest that the RGT, as the centerpiece of


assessment in Kelly’s PCT, is a practicable pedagogical strategy. Of benefit to both teachers


and students, the RGT affords an overall framework from which to organize course


content. Consistent with Tobacyk’s (1987) conclusions in evaluating the instructional


value of the RGT, the use of bipolar constructs “helps in achieving a more sophisticated


level of understanding than that obtained by the mere memorization of material” (p. 111).


In particular, the RGT encourages students to evaluate, compare, and contrast competing


intellectual perspectives. When conjoined with opportunities for classroom discussion,


the RGT also invites active participation in the learning process.

Free download pdf