Teaching Critical Thinking in Psychology: A Handbook of Best Practices

(ff) #1

Paul C. Smith & Kris Vasquez


214


What exactly is it that makes some of these beliefs fair game for research and critical


thinking although others are not? It is not easy to establish a clear divide: establishing exact


conditions of applicability for critical thinking and research raises some epistemological


questions that are still unresolved. Critical thinking-minded psychology faculty are fairly


good judges of the conditions of applicability of critical thinking and research skills, but


paradoxically use a sort of “I know it when I see it” heuristic rather than explicit rules.


They often have long experience with false claims and personal experience-based asser-


tions and also have long experience imagining and eliminating alternative explanations.


The expertise is in us, but it has not been made explicit. As a result, we do not have a good


method of teaching students how to discern between claims that really are supported by


certain personal experiences and those that are not. If we expect students to apply their


critical thinking skills in everyday life, we need to develop such a method.


Irresistible Forces

Another consistent difficulty in assignments asking for critical thinking from students is


that where their values are involved, their ability to think critically seems to be diminished.


Students engaging in discussion about a social issue may quickly revert to defensiveness or


ad hominem attacks. Although this behavior could be attributed to a lack of background


information or analytical skills, it is also possible that the nature of the topic itself impedes


critical thinking.


Consider an example assignment from research methods: teaching the distinction


between correlation and causation. In this assignment, students are asked to read press


coverage of an article discussing cognitive outcomes for children in day care, as well as


the original scholarly source (Brooks-Gunn, Han, & Waldfogel, 2002). Though the


class has covered extensively the difference between correlation and causation, though


the popular press uses the word “cause” in its headline, and though Brooks-Gunn and


colleagues explicitly say that their study does not show causal links, the students are


often unable to spot the key difference between the original article and the popular


press coverage.


In contrast, using Stanovich’s (2007) excellent example of the correlation in Taiwan


between number of small household electric appliances and use of contraception, the


students unfailingly see the folly of drawing causal conclusions from correlational data.


Yet somehow when it comes to the question of working mothers, some students appear


unable to make this critical distinction. Instead, critical consideration of the validity of


the causal conclusions gives way to an emotionally charged voicing of opinions about the


economic factors in society that drive women with young children into the workforce,


about the sexism inherent in the assumption that mothers are solely responsible for child


rearing, and about the selfishness of mothers who won’t “do what’s right” and stay home


with their children.


Similar posturing, without critical thought, can be produced by topics such as the


basis of homosexual orientation or the role of evolution in mate selection. It is easy to


assume that this lack of rational exchange reflects insufficient reasoning skills.

Free download pdf